Return to site

Add a Blog Post Title

broken image

 

 

 

Democratic leadership, also known as participative leadership, is a type of leadership style in which members of the group take a more participative role in the decision-making process. Researchers have found that this learning style is usually one of the most effective and lead to higher productivity. Feb 10, 2017  The business decision making process is commonly divided into seven steps. Managers may utilize many of these steps without realizing it, but gaining a clearer understanding of best practices can improve the effectiveness of your decisions. Steps of the Decision Making Process. The following are the seven key steps of the decision making process. Types of Decision Making - An Overview We determine types of decision making by looking at outcomes and the impacted entity. At the highest level we have chosen to categorize decisions into three major types: consumer decision making, business decision making, and personal decision making.

*Decision Making Process Steps Pdf

*The Decision Making Process

*What Is Decision Making Process

*Types Of Decision Making

*Types Of Decision Making Models

*Types Of Decision Making Process Pdf Download

*Project Management Concepts

*Useful Resource

*Selected ReadingIntroduction

Decision making is a daily activity for any human being. There is no exception about that. When it comes to business organizations, decision making is a habit and a process as well.

Effective and successful decisions make profit to the company and unsuccessful ones make losses. Therefore, corporate decision making process is the most critical process in any organization.

In the decision making process, we choose one course of action from a few possible alternatives. In the process of decision making, we may use many tools, techniques and perceptions.

In addition, we may make our own private decisions or may prefer a collective decision.

Usually, decision making is hard. Majority of corporate decisions involve some level of dissatisfaction or conflict with another party.

Let's have a look at the decision making process in detail.Steps of Decision Making Process

Following are the important steps of the decision making process. Each step may be supported by different tools and techniques.Step 1: Identification of the purpose of the decision

In this step, the problem is thoroughly analysed. There are a couple of questions one should ask when it comes to identifying the purpose of the decision.

*

What exactly is the problem?

*

Why the problem should be solved?

*

Who are the affected parties of the problem?

*

Does the problem have a deadline or a specific time-line?Step 2: Information gathering

A problem of an organization will have many stakeholders. How to download movies from watch movies netflix on mac without app. In addition, there can be dozens of factors involved and affected by the problem.

In the process of solving the problem, you will have to gather as much as information related to the factors and stakeholders involved in the problem. For the process of information gathering, tools such as 'Check Sheets' can be effectively used.Step 3: Principles for judging the alternatives

In this step, the baseline criteria for judging the alternatives should be set up. When it comes to defining the criteria, organizational goals as well as the corporate culture should be taken into consideration.

As an example, profit is one of the main concerns in every decision making process. Companies usually do not make decisions that reduce profits, unless it is an exceptional case. Likewise, baseline principles should be identified related to the problem in hand.Step 4: Brainstorm and analyse the different choices

For this step, brainstorming to list down all the ideas is the best option. Before the idea generation step, it is vital to understand the causes of the problem and prioritization of causes.

For this, you can make use of Cause-and-Effect diagrams and Pareto Chart tool. Cause-and-Effect diagram helps you to identify all possible causes of the problem and Pareto chart helps you to prioritize and identify the causes with highest effect.

Then, you can move on generating all possible solutions (alternatives) for the problem in hand.Step 5: Evaluation of alternatives

Use your judgement principles and decision-making criteria to evaluate each alternative. In this step, experience and effectiveness of the judgement principles come into play. You need to compare each alternative for their positives and negatives.Step 6: Select the best alternative

Once you go through from Step 1 to Step 5, this step is easy. In addition, the selection of the best alternative is an informed decision since you have already followed a methodology to derive and select the best alternative.Step 7: Execute the decision

Convert your decision into a plan or a sequence of activities. Execute your plan by yourself or with the help of subordinates.Step 8: Evaluate the results

Evaluate the outcome of your decision. See whether there is anything you should learn and then correct in future decision making. This is one of the best practices that will improve your decision-making skills.Conclusion

When it comes to making decisions, one should always weigh the positive and negative business consequences and should favour the positive outcomes.

This avoids the possible losses to the organization and keeps the company running with a sustained growth. Sometimes, avoiding decision making seems easier; especially, when you get into a lot of confrontation after making the tough decision.

But, making the decisions and accepting its consequences is the only way to stay in control of your corporate life and time.Sample flowchart representing a decision process to add a new article to Wikipedia.

In psychology, decision-making (also spelled decision making and decisionmaking) is regarded as the cognitive process resulting in the selection of a belief or a course of action among several alternative possibilities. Decision-making is the process of identifying and choosing alternatives based on the values, preferences and beliefs of the decision-maker. Every decision-making process produces a final choice, which may or may not prompt action.

Research about decision-making is also published under the label problem solving, in particular in European psychological research.[1]

*2Problem analysis

*3Neuroscience

*4Decision-making techniques

*5Steps

*10Cognitive stylesOverview[edit]

Decision-making can be regarded as a problem-solving activity yielding a solution deemed to be optimal, or at least satisfactory. It is therefore a process which can be more or less rational or irrational and can be based on explicit or tacit knowledge and beliefs. Tacit knowledge is often used to fill the gaps in complex decision making processes.[2] Usually both of these types of knowledge, tacit and explicit, are used together in the decision-making process.

Human performance has been the subject of active research from several perspectives:

Killing floor download free. full version. The game utilizes a heavily modified version of Epic Games’ Unreal Engine 3.

*Psychological: examining individual decisions in the context of a set of needs, preferences and values the individual has or seeks.

*Cognitive: the decision-making process regarded as a continuous process integrated in the interaction with the environment.

*Normative: the analysis of individual decisions concerned with the logic of decision-making, or communicative rationality, and the invariant choice it leads to.[3]

A major part of decision-making involves the analysis of a finite set of alternatives described in terms of evaluative criteria. Then the task might be to rank these alternatives in terms of how attractive they are to the decision-maker(s) when all the criteria are considered simultaneously. Another task might be to find the best alternative or to determine the relative total priority of each alternative (for instance, if alternatives represent projects competing for funds) when all the criteria are considered simultaneously. Solving such problems is the focus of multiple-criteria decision analysis (MCDA). This area of decision-making, although very old, has attracted the interest of many researchers and practitioners and is still highly debated as there are many MCDA methods which may yield very different results when they are applied on exactly the same data.[4] This leads to the formulation of a decision-making paradox. Logical decision-making is an important part of all science-based professions, where specialists apply their knowledge in a given area to make informed decisions. For example, medical decision-making often involves a diagnosis and the selection of appropriate treatment. But naturalistic decision-making research shows that in situations with higher time pressure, higher stakes, or increased ambiguities, experts may use intuitive decision-making rather than structured approaches. They may follow a recognition primed decision that fits their experience and arrive at a course of action without weighing alternatives.[citation needed]

The decision-maker's environment can play a part in the decision-making process. For example, environmental complexity is a factor that influences cognitive function.[5] A complex environment is an environment with a large number of different possible states which come and go over time.[6] Studies done at the University of Colorado have shown that more complex environments correlate with higher cognitive function, which means that a decision can be influenced by the location. One experiment measured complexity in a room by the number of small objects and appliances present; a simple room had less of those things. Cognitive function was greatly affected by the higher measure of environmental complexity making it easier to think about the situation and make a better decision.[5]Problem analysis[edit]

It is important to differentiate between problem analysis and decision-making. Traditionally, it is argued that problem analysis must be done first, so that the information gathered in that process may be used towards decision-making.[7][page needed]Characteristics of problem analysis

*Problems are merely deviations from performance standards

*Problems must be precisely identified and described

*Problems are caused by a change from a distinctive feature

*Something can always be used to distinguish between what has and hasn't been affected by a cause

*Causes of problems can be deduced from relevant changes found in analyzing the problem

*Most likely cause of a problem is the one that exactly explains all the facts, while having the fewest (or weakest) assumptions (Occam's razor).Characteristics of decision-making

*Objectives must first be established

*Objectives must be classified and placed in order of importance

*Alternative actions must be developed

*The alternatives must be evaluated against all the objectives

*The alternative that is able to achieve all the objectives is the tentative decision

*The tentative decision is evaluated for more possible consequences

*The decisive actions are taken, and additional actions are taken to prevent any adverse consequences from becoming problems and starting both systems (problem analysis and decision-making) all over again

*There are steps that are generally followed that result in a decision model that can be used to determine an optimal production plan[8]

*In a situation featuring conflict, role-playing may be helpful for predicting decisions to be made by involved parties[9]Analysis paralysis[edit]

Analysis paralysis is the state of over-analyzing (or over-thinking) a situation so that a decision or action is never taken, in effect paralyzing the outcome.[10]Information overload[edit]

Information overload is 'a gap between the volume of information and the tools we have to assimilate' it.[11] Information used in decision making is to reduce or eliminate uncertainty.[12] Excessive information affects problem processing and tasking, which affects decision-making.[13] Crystal C. Hall and colleagues described an 'illusion of knowledge', which means that as individuals encounter too much knowledge it can interfere with their ability to make rational decisions.[14]Post-decision analysis[edit]

Evaluation and analysis of past decisions is complementary to decision-making. See also Mental accounting and Postmortem documentation.Neuroscience[edit]

Decision-making is a region of intense study in the fields of systems neuroscience, and cognitive neuroscience. Several brain structures, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex and the overlapping ventromedial prefrontal cortex are believed to be involved in decision-making processes. A neuroimaging study[15] found distinctive patterns of neural activation in these regions depending on whether decisions were made on the basis of perceived personal volition or following directions from someone else. Patients with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex have difficulty making advantageous decisions.[16][page needed]

A common laboratory paradigm for studying neural decision-making is the two-alternative forced choice task (2AFC), in which a subject has to choose between two alternatives within a certain time. A study of a two-alternative forced choice task involving rhesus monkeys found that neurons in the parietal cortex not only represent the formation of a decision[17] but also signal the degree of certainty (or 'confidence') associated with the decision.[18] Another recent study found that lesions to the ACC in the macaque resulted in impaired decision-making in the long run of reinforcement guided tasks suggesting that the ACC may be involved in evaluating past reinforcement information and guiding future action.[19] A 2012 study found that rats and humans can optimally accumulate incoming sensory evidence, to make statistically optimal decisions.[20]Emotions[edit]

Emotion appears able to aid the decision-making process. Decision-making often occurs in the face of uncertainty about whether one's choices will lead to benefit or harm (see also Risk). The somatic marker hypothesis is a neurobiological theory of how decisions are made in the face of uncertain outcome.[21] This theory holds that such decisions are aided by emotions, in the form of bodily states, that are elicited during the deliberation of future consequences and that mark different options for behavior as being advantageous or disadvantageous. This process involves an interplay between neural systems that elicit emotional/bodily states and neural systems that map these emotional/bodily states.[22] A recent lesion mapping study of 152 patients with focal brain lesions conducted by Aron K. Barbey and colleagues provided evidence to help discover the neural mechanisms of emotional intelligence.[23][24][25]Decision-making techniques[edit]

Decision-making techniques can be separated into two broad categories: group decision-making techniques and individual decision-making techniques. Individual decision-making techniques can also often be applied by a group.Group[edit]

*Consensus decision-making tries to avoid 'winners' and 'losers'. Consensus requires that a majority approve a given course of action, but that the minority agree to go along with the course of action. In other words, if the minority opposes the course of action, consensus requires that the course of action be modified to remove objectionable features.

*Voting-based methods:

*Majority requires support from more than 50% of the members of the group. Thus, the bar for action is lower than with consensus.

*Plurality, where the largest block in a group decides, even if it falls short of a majority.

*Quadratic voting allows participants to cast their preference and intensity of preference for each decision (as opposed to a simple for or against decision). It addresses issues of voting paradox and majority-rule.

*Range voting lets each member score one or more of the available options. The option with the highest average is chosen. This method has experimentally been shown to produce the lowest Bayesian regret among common voting methods, even when voters are strategic.[citation needed]

*Delphi method is a structured communication technique for groups, originally developed for collaborative forecasting but has also been used for policy making.

*Dotmocracy is a facilitation method that relies on the use of special forms called Dotmocracy Sheets to allow large groups to collectively brainstorm and recognize agreement on an unlimited number of ideas they have authored.

*Participative decision-making occurs when an authority opens up the decision-making process to a group of people for a collaborative effort.

*Decision engineering uses a visual map of the decision-making process based on system dynamics and can be automated through a decision modeling tool, integrating big data, machine learning, and expert knowledge as appropriate.Individual[edit]

*Decisional balance sheet: listing the advantages and disadvantages (benefits and costs, pros and cons) of each option, as suggested by Plato's Protagoras and by Benjamin Franklin.[26]

*Expected-value optimization: choosing the alternative with the highest probability-weighted utility, possibly with some consideration for risk aversion. This may involve considering the opportunity cost of different alternatives. See also Decision analysis and Decision theory.

*Satisficing: examining alternatives only until the first acceptable one is found. The opposite is maximizing or optimizing, in which many or all alternatives are examined in order to find the best option.

*Acquiesce to a person in authority or an 'expert'; 'just following orders'.

*Anti-authoritarianism: taking the most opposite action compared to the advice of mistrusted authorities.

*Flipism e.g. flipping a coin, cutting a deck of playing cards, and other random or coincidence methods – or prayer, tarot cards, astrology, augurs, revelation, or other forms of divination, superstition or pseudoscience.

*Automated decision support: setting up criteria for automated decisions.

*Decision support systems: using decision-making software when faced with highly complex decisions or when considering many stakeholders, categories, or other factors that affect decisions.Steps[edit]

A variety of researchers have formulated similar prescriptive steps aimed at improving decision-making.GOFER[edit]

In the 1980s, psychologist Leon Mann and colleagues developed a decision-making process called GOFER, which they taught to adolescents, as summarized in the book Teaching Decision Making To Adolescents.[27] The process was based on extensive earlier research conducted with psychologist Irving Janis.[28] GOFER is an acronym for five decision-making steps:[29]

*Goals clarification: Survey values and objectives.

*Options generation: Consider a wide range of alternative actions.

*Facts-finding: Search for information.

*Consideration of Effects: Weigh the positive and negative consequences of the options.

*Review and implementation: Plan how to review the options and implement them.DECIDE[edit]

In 2008, Kristina Guo published the DECIDE model of decision-making, which has six parts:[30]

*Define the problem

*Establish or Enumerate all the criteria (constraints)

*Consider or Collect all the alternatives

*Identify the best alternative

*Develop and implement a plan of action

*Evaluate and monitor the solution and examine feedback when necessaryOther[edit]

In 2007, Pam Brown of Singleton Hospital in Swansea, Wales, divided the decision-making process into seven steps:[31]

*Outline the goal and outcome.

*Gather data.

*Develop alternatives (i.e., brainstorming).

*List pros and cons of each alternative.

*Make the decision.

*Immediately take action to implement it.

*Learn from and reflect on the decision.

In 2009, professor John Pijanowski described how the Arkansas Program, an ethics curriculum at the University of Arkansas, used eight stages of moral decision-making based on the work of James Rest:[32]:6

*Establishing community: Create and nurture the relationships, norms, and procedures that will influence how problems are understood and communicated. This stage takes place prior to and during a moral dilemma.

*Perception: Recognize that a problem exists.

*Interpretation: Identify competing explanations for the problem, and evaluate the drivers behind those interpretations.

*Judgment: Sift through various possible actions or responses and determine which is more justifiable.

*Motivation: Examine the competing commitments which may distract from a more moral course of action and then prioritize and commit to moral values over other personal, institutional or social values.

*Action: Follow through with action that supports the more justified decision.

*Reflection in action.

*Reflection on action.Group stages[edit]

According to B. Aubrey Fisher, there are four stages or phases that should be involved in all group decision-making:[33]Decision Making Process Steps Pdf

*Orientation. Members meet for the first time and start to get to know each other.

*Conflict. Once group members become familiar with each other, disputes, little fights and arguments occur. Group members eventually work it out.

*Emergence. The group begins to clear up vague opinions by talking about them.

*Reinforcement. Members finally make a decision and provide justification for it.

It is said that establishing critical norms in a group improves the quality of decisions, while the majority of opinions (called consensus norms) do not.[34]

Conflicts in socialization are divided in to functional and dysfunctional types. Functional conflicts are mostly the questioning the managers assumptions in their decision making and dysfunctional conflicts are like personal attacks and every action which decrease team effectiveness. Functional conflicts are the better ones to gain higher quality decision making caused by the increased team knowledge and shared understanding.[35]Rational and irrational[edit]

In economics, it is thought that if humans are rational and free to make their own decisions, then they would behave according to rational choice theory.[36]:368–370 Rational choice theory says that a person consistently makes choices that lead to the best situation for himself or herself, taking into account all available considerations including costs and benefits; the rationality of these considerations is from the point of view of the person himself, so a decision is not irrational just because someone else finds it questionable.

In reality, however, there are some factors that affect decision-making abilities and cause people to make irrational decisions – for example, to make contradictory choices when faced with the same problem framed in two different ways (see also Allais paradox).

One of the most prominent theories of decision making is subjective expected utility (SEU) theory, which describes the rational behavior of the decision maker.[37] The decision maker assesses different alternatives by their utilities and the subjective probability of occurrence.[37]

Rational decision-making is often grounded on experience and theories exist that are able to put this approach on solid mathematical grounds so that subjectivity is reduced to a minimum, see e.g. scenario optimization.In adolescents vs. adults[edit]

During their adolescent years, teens are known for their high-risk behaviors and rash decisions. Recent research[citation needed] has shown that there are differences in cognitive processes between adolescents and adults during decision-making. Researchers have concluded that differences in decision-making are not due to a lack of logic or reasoning, but more due to the immaturity of psychosocial capacities that influence decision-making. Examples of their undeveloped capacities which influence decision-making would be impulse control, emotion regulation, delayed gratification and resistance to peer pressure. In the past, researchers have thought that adolescent behavior was simply due to incompetency regarding decision-making. Currently, researchers have concluded that adults and adolescents are both competent decision-makers, not just adults. However, adolescents' competent decision-making skills decrease when psychosocial capacities become present.

Recent research[citation needed] has shown that risk-taking behaviors in adolescents may be the product of interactions between the socioemotional brain network and its cognitive-control network. The socioemotional part of the brain processes social and emotional stimuli and has been shown to be important in reward processing. The cognitive-control network assists in planning and self-regulation. Both of these sections of the brain change over the course of puberty. However, the socioemotional network changes quickly and abruptly, while the cognitive-control network changes more gradually. Because of this difference in change, the cognitive-control network, which usually regulates the socioemotional network, struggles to control the socioemotional network when psychosocial capacities are present.[clarification needed]

When adolescents are exposed to social and emotional stimuli, their socioemotional network is activated as well as areas of the brain involved in reward processing. Because teens often gain a sense of reward from risk-taking behaviors, their repetition becomes ever more probable due to the reward experienced. In this, the process mirrors addiction. Teens can become addicted to risky behavior because they are in a high state of arousal and are rewarded for it not only by their own internal functions but also by their peers around them.

Adults are generally better able to control their risk-taking because their cognitive-control system has matured enough to the point where it can control the socioemotional network, even in the context of high arousal or when psychosocial capacities are present. Also, adults are less likely to find themselves in situations that push them to do risky things. For example, teens are more likely to be around peers who peer pressure them into doing things, while adults are not as exposed to this sort of social setting.[38][39]

A recent study suggests that adolescents have difficulties adequately adjusting beliefs in response to bad news (such as reading that smoking poses a greater risk to health than they thought), but do not differ from adults in their ability to alter beliefs in response to good news.[40] This creates biased beliefs, which may lead to greater risk taking.[41]The Decision Making ProcessCognitive and personal biases[edit]

Biases usually affect decision-making processes. They appear more when decision task has time pressure, is done under high stress and/or task is highly complex. [42]

Here is a list of commonly debated biases in judgment and decision-making:

*Selective search for evidence (also known as confirmation bias): People tend to be willing to gather facts that support certain conclusions but disregard other facts that support different conclusions. Individuals who are highly defensive in this manner show significantly greater left prefrontal cortex activity as measured by EEG than do less defensive individuals.[43]

*Premature termination of search for evidence: People tend to accept the first alternative that looks like it might work.

*Cognitive inertia is the unwillingness to change existing thought patterns in the face of new circumstances.

*Selective perception: People actively screen out information that they do not think is important (see also Prejudice). In one demonstration of this effect, discounting of arguments with which one disagrees (by judging them as untrue or irrelevant) was decreased by selective activation of right prefrontal cortex.[44]

*Wishful thinking is a tendency to want to see things in a certain – usually positive – light, which can distort perception and thinking.[45]

*Choice-supportive bias occurs when people distort their memories of chosen and rejected options to make the chosen options seem more attractive.

*Recency: People tend to place more attention on more recent information and either ignore or forget more distant information (see Semantic priming). The opposite effect in the first set of data or other information is termed primacy effect.[46][page needed]

*Repetition bias is a willingness to believe what one has been told most often and by the greatest number of different sources.

*Anchoring and adjustment: Decisions are unduly influenced by initial information that shapes our view of subsequent information.

*Groupthink is peer pressure to conform to the opinions held by the group.

*Source credibility bias is a tendency to reject a person's statement on the basis of a bias against the person, organization, or group to which the person belongs. People preferentially accept statement by others that they like (see also Prejudice).

*Incremental decision-making and escalating commitment: People look at a decision as a small step in a process, and this tends to perpetuate a series of similar decisions. This can be contrasted with zero-based decision-making (see Slippery slope).

*Attribution asymmetry: People tend to attribute their own success to internal factors, including abilities and talents, but explain their failures in terms of external factors such as bad luck. The reverse bias is shown when people explain others' success or failure.

*Role fulfillment is a tendency to conform to others' decision-making expectations.

*Underestimating uncertainty and the illusion of control: People tend to underestimate future uncertainty because of a tendency to believe they have more control over events than they really do.

*Framing bias: This is best avoided by increasing numeracy and presenting data in several formats (for example, using both absolute and relative scales).[47]

*Sunk-cost fallacy is a specific type of framing effect that affects decision-making. It involves an individual making a decision about a current situation based on what they have previously invested in the situation.[36]:372 An example of this would be an individual that is refraining from dropping a class that they are most likely to fail, due to the fact that they feel as though they have done so much work in the course thus far.

*Prospect theory involves the idea that when faced with a decision-making event, an individual is more likely to take on a risk when evaluating potential losses, and are more likely to avoid risks when evaluating potential gains. This can influence one's decision-making depending if the situation entails a threat, or opportunity.[36]:373

*Optimism bias is a tendency to overestimate the likelihood of positive events occurring in the future and underestimate the likelihood of negative life events.[48] Such biased expectations are generated and maintained in the face of counter-evidence through a tendency to discount undesirable information.[49] An optimism bias can alter risk perception and decision-making in many domains, ranging from finance to health.

*Reference class forecasting was developed to eliminate or reduce cognitive biases in decision-making.Cognitive limitations in groups[edit]

In groups, people generate decisions through active and complex processes. One method consists of three steps: initial preferences are expressed by members; the members of the group then gather and share information concerning those preferences; finally, the members combine their views and make a single choice about how to face the problem. Although these steps are relatively ordinary, judgements are often distorted by cognitive and motivational biases, include 'sins of commission', 'sins of omission', and 'sins of imprecision'.[50][page needed]Cognitive styles[edit]Optimizing vs. satisficing[edit]

Herbert A. Simon coined the phrase 'bounded rationality' to express the idea that human decision-making is limited by available information, available time and the mind's information-processing ability. Further psychological research has identified individual differences between two cognitive styles: maximizers try to make an optimal decision, whereas satisficers simply try to find a solution that is 'good enough'. Maximizers tend to take longer making decisions due to the need to maximize performance across all variables and make tradeoffs carefully; they also tend to more often regret their decisions (perhaps because they are more able than satisficers to recognise that a decision turned out to be sub-optimal).[51]What Is Decision Making ProcessIntuitive vs. rational[edit]

The psychologist Daniel Kahneman, adopting terms originally proposed by the psychologists Keith Stanovich and Richard West, has theorized that a person's decision-making is the result of an interplay between two kinds of cognitive processes: an automatic intuitive system (called 'System 1') and an effortful rational system (called 'System 2'). System 1 is a bottom-up, fast, and implicit system of decision-making, while system 2 is a top-down, slow, and explicit system of decision-making.[52] System 1 includes simple heuristics in judgment and decision-making such as the affect heuristic, the availability heuristic, the familiarity heuristic, and the representativeness heuristic.Combinatorial vs. positional[edit]

Styles and methods of decision-making were elaborated by Aron Katsenelinboigen, the founder of predispositioning theory. In his analysis on styles and methods, Katsenelinboigen referred to the game of chess, saying that 'chess does disclose various methods of operation, notably the creation of predisposition-methods which may be applicable to other, more complex systems.'[53]:5

Katsenelinboigen states that apart from the methods (reactive and selective) and sub-methods (randomization, predispositioning, programming), there are two major styles: positional and combinational. Both styles are utilized in the game of chess. According to Katsenelinboigen, the two styles reflect two basic approaches to uncertainty: deterministic (combinational style) and indeterministic (positional style). Katsenelinboigen's definition of the two styles are the following.

The combinational style is characterized by:

*a very narrow, clearly defined, primarily material goal; and

*a program that links the initial position with the final outcome.

In defining the combinational style in chess, Katsenelinboigen wrote: 'The combinational style features a clearly formulated limited objective, namely the capture of material (the main constituent element of a chess position). The objective is implemented via a well-defined, and in some cases, unique sequence of moves aimed at reaching the set goal. As a rule, this sequence leaves no options for the opponent. Finding a combinational objective allows the player to focus all his energies on efficient execution, that is, the player's analysis may be limited to the pieces directly partaking in the combination. This approach is the crux of the combination and the combinational style of play.[53]:57

The positional style is distinguished by:

*a positional goal; and

*a formation of semi-complete linkages between the initial step and final outcome.Types Of Decision Making

'Unlike the combinational player, the positional player is occupied, first and foremost, with the elaboration of the position that will allow him to develop in the unknown future. In playing the positional style, the player must evaluate relational and material parameters as independent variables. .. The positional style gives the player the opportunity to develop a position until it becomes pregnant with a combination. However, the combination is not the final goal of the positional player – it helps him to achieve the desirable, keeping in mind a predisposition for the future development. The pyrrhic victory is the best example of one's inability to think positionally.'[54]

The positional style serves to:

*create a predisposition to the future development of the position;

*induce the environment in a certain way;

*absorb an unexpected outcome in one's favor; and

*avoid the negative aspects of unexpected outcomes.Influence of Myers-Briggs type[edit]

According to Isabel Briggs Myers, a person's decision-making process depends to a significant degree on their cognitive style.[55][page needed] Myers developed a set of four bi-polar dimensions, called the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). The terminal points on these dimensions are: thinking and feeling; extroversion and introversion; judgment and perception; and sensing and intuition. She claimed that a person's decision-making style correlates well with how they score on these four dimensions. For example, someone who scored near the thinking, extroversion, sensing, and judgment ends of the dimensions would tend to have a logical, analytical, objective, critical, and empirical decision-making style. However, some psychologists say that the MBTI lacks reliability and validity and is poorly constructed.[56][57]

Other studies suggest that these national or cross-cultural differences in decision-making exist across entire societies. For example, Maris Martinsons has found that American, Japanese and Chinese business leaders each exhibit a distinctive national style of decision-making.[58]General decision-making style (GDMS)[edit]

In the general decision-making style (GDMS) test developed by Suzanne Scott and Reginald Bruce, there are five decision-making styles: rational, intuitive, dependent, avoidant, and spontaneous.[59][60] These five different decision-making styles change depending on the context and situation, and one style is not necessarily better than any other. In the examples below, the individual is working for a company and is offered a job from a different company.

*The rational style is an in-depth search for, and a strong consideration of, other options and/or information prior to making a decision. In this style, the individual would research the new job being offered, review their current job, and look at the pros and cons of taking the new job versus staying with their current company.

*The intuitive style is confidence in one's initial feelings and gut reactions. In this style, if the individual initially prefers the new job because they have a feeling that the work environment is better suited for them, then they would decide to take the new job. The individual might not make this decision as soon as the job is offered.

*The dependent style is asking for other people's input and instructions on what decision should be made. In this style, the individual could ask friends, family, coworkers, etc., but the individual might not ask all of these people.

*The avoidant style is averting the responsibility of making a decision. In this style, the individual would not make a decision. Therefore, the individual would stick with their current job.

*The spontaneous style is a need to make a decision as soon as possible rather than waiting to make a decision. In this style, the individual would either reject or accept the job as soon as it is offered.See also[edit]Library resources about 

Decision-makingTypes Of Decision Making ModelsReferences[edit]Types Of Decision Making Process Pdf Download

*^Frensch, Peter A.; Funke, Joachim, eds. (1995). Complex problem solving: the European perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN978-0805813364. OCLC32131412.

*^Brockmann, Erich N.; Anthony, William P. (December 2016). 'Tacit knowledge and strategic decision making'. Group & Organization Management. 27 (4): 436–455. doi:10.1177/1059601102238356.

*^Kahneman, Daniel; Tversky, Amos, eds. (2000). Choices, values, and frames. New York; Cambridge, UK: Russell Sage Foundation; Cambridge University Press. p. 211. ISBN978-0521621724. OCLC42934579.

*^Triantaphyllou, Evangelos (2000). Multi-criteria decision making methods: a comparative study. Applied optimization. 44. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. p. 320. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-3157-6. ISBN978-0792366072.

*^ abDavidson, Alice Ware; Bar-Yam, Yaneer (2006) [2000]. 'Environmental complexity: information for human–environment well-being'(PDF). In Bar-Yam, Yaneer; Minai, Ali (eds.). Unifying themes in complex systems. Berlin; New York: Springer. pp. 157–168. CiteSeerX10.1.1.33.7118. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-35866-4_16. ISBN9783540358640.

*^Godfrey-Smith, Peter (2001). 'Environmental complexity and the evolution of cognition'(PDF). In Sternberg, Robert J.; Kaufman, James C. (eds.). The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 223–250. ISBN978-0805832679. OCLC44775038.

*^Kepner, Charles Higgins; Tregoe, Benjamin B. (1997) [1965]. The new rational manager: an updated edition for a new world (Updated ed.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Research Press. OCLC37666447.

*^Monahan, George E. (2000). Management decision making: spreadsheet modeling, analysis, and application. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 33–40. ISBN978-0521781183. OCLC42921287.

*^Armstrong, Jon Scott (2001). 'Role playing: a method to forecast decisions'. In Armstrong, Jon Scott (ed.). Principles of forecasting: a handbook for researchers and practitioners. International series in operations research & management science. 30. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 15–30. CiteSeerX10.1.1.464.5677. doi:10.1007/978-0-306-47630-3_2. ISBN978-0792379300.

*^'analysis paralysis | Definition of analysis paralysis in US English by Oxford Dictionaries'. Oxford Dictionaries | English. Retrieved 2018-11-10.

*^Paul Saffo quoted in: Foley, John (30 October 1995). 'Managing information: infoglut'. InformationWeek. Archived from the original on 2001-02-22. Retrieved 2015-07-26.

*^Duncan (1972). 'Characteristics of organizational environments and perceived environment uncertainty'. Administrative Science Quarterly. 17 (3): 313–27. doi:10.2307/2392145. JSTOR2392145.

*^Kutty, Ambalika D.; Kumar Shee, Himanshu; Pathak, R. D. (November 2007). 'Decision-making: too much info!'. Monash Business Review. 3 (3): 8–9. doi:10.2104/mbr07056.[permanent dead link]

*^Hall, Crystal C.; Ariss, Lynn; Todorov, Alexander (July 2007). 'The illusion of knowledge: when more information reduces accuracy and increases confidence'(PDF). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 103 (2): 277–290. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.003.

*^Walton, Mark E.; Devlin, Joseph T.; Rushworth, Matthew F. S. (November 2004). 'Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex'. Nature Neuroscience. 7 (11): 1259–1265. doi:10.1038/nn1339. PMID15494729.

*^Damasio, Antonio R. (1994). Descartes' error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam. ISBN978-0399138942. OCLC30780083.

*^Gold, Joshua I.; Shadlen, Michael N. (2007). 'The neural basis of decision making'. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 30: 535–574. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. PMID17600525.

*^Kiani, Roozbeh; Shadlen, Michael N. (May 2009). 'Representation of confidence associated with a decision by neurons in the parietal cortex'. Science. 324 (5928): 759–764. doi:10.1126/science.1169405. PMC2738936. PMID19423820.

*^Kennerley, Steven W.; Walton, Mark E.; Behrens, Timothy E. J.; Buckley, Mark J.; Rushworth, Matthew F. S. (July 2006). 'Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex'. Nature Neuroscience. 9 (7): 940–947. doi:10.1038/nn1724. PMID16783368.

*^Brunton, Bingni W.; Botvinick, Matthew M.; Brody, Carlos D. (April 2013). 'Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making'(PDF). Science. 340 (6128): 95–98. doi:10.1126/science.1233912. PMID23559254. Archived from the original(PDF) on 2016-03-05.

*^Reimann, Martin; Bechara, Antoine (October 2010). 'The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research'. Journal of Economic Psychology. 31 (5): 767–776. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2010.03.002.

*^Naqvi, Nasir; Shiv, Baba; Bechara, Antoine (October 2006). 'The role of emotion in decision making: a cognitive neuroscience perspective'. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (5): 260–264. CiteSeerX10.1.1.137.4677. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00448.x.

*^Barbey, Aron K.; Colom, Roberto; Grafman, Jordan (March 2014). 'Distributed neural system for emotional intelligence revealed by lesion mapping'. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 9 (3): 265–272. doi:10.1093/scan/nss124. PMC3980800. PMID23171618.

*^Yates, Diana. 'Researchers map emotional intelligence in the brain'. University of Illinois News Bureau. University of Illinois.

*^HealthDay (2013-01-28). 'Scientists complete 1st map of 'emotional intelligence' in the brain'. US News and World Report.

*^Franklin, Benjamin (1975) [1772]. 'To Joseph Priestley'. In Willcox, William Bradford (ed.). The papers of Benjamin Franklin: January 1 through December 31, 1772. 19. New Haven: Yale University Press. pp. 299–300. ISBN978-0300018653. OCLC310601.

*^Mann, Leon; Harmoni, Ros; Power, Colin (1991). 'The GOFER course in decision making'. In Baron, Jonathan; Brown, Rex V. (eds.). Teaching decision making to adolescents. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 61–78. ISBN978-0805804973. OCLC22507012. See also: Mann, Leon (July 1989). 'Becoming a better decision maker'. Australian Psychologist. 24 (2): 141–155. doi:10.1080/00050068908259558.

*^Janis, Irving L.; Mann, Leon (1977). Decision making: a psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press. ISBN978-0029161609. OCLC2542340.

*^Mann, Leon; Harmoni, Ros; Power, Colin; Beswick, Gery; Ormond, Cheryl (July 1988). 'Effectiveness of the GOFER course in decision making for high school students'. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 1 (3): 159–168. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960010304.

*^Guo, Kristina L. (June 2008). 'DECIDE: a decision-making model for more effective decision making by health care managers'. The Health Care Manager. 27 (2): 118–127. doi:10.1097/01.HCM.0000285046.27290.90. PMID18475113.

*^Brown, Pam (November 29, 2007), Career coach: decision-making, Pulse, retrieved July 12, 2012(subscription required)

*^Pijanowski, John (February 2009). 'The role of learning theory in building effective college ethics curricula'. Journal of College and Character. 10 (3): 1–13. doi:10.2202/1940-1639.1088.

*^Griffin, Emory A. (1991). 'Interact system model of decision emergence of B. Aubrey Fisher'(PDF). A first look at communication theory (1st ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 253–262. ISBN978-0070227781. OCLC21973427.

*^Postmes, T; Spears, Russell; Cihangir, Sezgin (2001). 'Quality of decision making and group norms'. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 918–930. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.918. PMID11414374.

*^ Brockmann, E., Anthony, W., 2002. Tacit knowledge and strategic decision making. Group & Organization Management. Vol. 27 (4), 436–455. doi:10.1177/1059601102238356

*^ abcSchacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel Todd; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011) [2009]. Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. ISBN9781429237192. OCLC755079969.

*^ abCrozier, W. Ray; Ranyard, Rob (1997). 'Cognitive process models and explanations of decision making'. In Ranyard, Rob; Crozier, W. Ray; Svenson, Ola (eds.). Decision making: cognitive models and explanations. Frontiers of cognitive science. London; New York: Routledge. pp. 5–20. ISBN978-0415158183. OCLC37043834.

*^Gardner, Margo; Steinberg, Laurence (July 2005). 'Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study'(PDF). Developmental Psychology. 41 (4): 625–635. CiteSeerX10.1.1.556.4973. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. PMID16060809.

*^Steinberg, Laurence (April 2007). 'Risk taking in adolescence: new perspectives from brain and behavioral science'. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (2): 55–59. CiteSeerX10.1.1.519.7099. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x.

*^Moutsiana, Christina; Garrett, Neil; Clarke, Richard C.; Lotto, R. Beau; Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne; Sharot, Tali (October 2013). 'Human development of the ability to learn from bad news'. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (41): 16396–16401. doi:10.1073/pnas.1305631110. PMC3799330. PMID24019466.

*^Reyna, Valerie F. (November 2013). 'Psychology: Good and bad news on the adolescent brain'. Nature. 503 (7474): 48–49. doi:10.1038/nature12704. PMID24172899.

*^T, Maqsood; A, Finegan; D, Walker (2004). 'Biases and heuristics in judgment and decision making: The dark side of tacit knowledge'. Issues in Informing Science and Information Technology. 1. ISSN1547-5840.

*^Blackhart, G. C.; Kline, J. P. (2005). 'Individual differences in anterior EEG asymmetry between high and low defensive individuals during a rumination/distraction task'. Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (2): 427–437. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.027.

*^Drake, R. A. (1993). 'Processing persuasive arguments: 2. Discounting of truth and relevance as a function of agreement and manipulated activation asymmetry'. Journal of Research in Personality. 27 (2): 184–196. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1993.1013.

*^Chua, E. F.; Rand-Giovannetti, E.; Schacter, D. L.; Albert, M.; Sperling, R. A. (2004). 'Dissociating confidence and accuracy: Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows origins of the subjective memory experience'(PDF). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16 (7): 1131–1142. doi:10.1162/0898929041920568. PMID15453969.

*^Plous, Scott (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. ISBN978-0877229131. OCLC26548229.

*^Perneger, Thomas V.; Agoritsas, Thomas (December 2011). 'Doctors and patients' susceptibility to framing bias: a randomized trial'. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 26 (12): 1411–1417. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1810-x. PMC3235613. PMID21792695.

*^Sharot, Tali (2011). The optimism bias: a tour of the irrationally positive brain (1st ed.). New York: Pantheon Books. ISBN9780307378484. OCLC667609433.

*^Sharot, Tali; Korn, Christoph W.; Dolan, Raymond J. (October 2011). 'How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality'. Nature Neuroscience. 14 (11): 1475–1479. doi:10.1038/nn.2949. PMC3204264. PMID21983684.

*^Forsyth, Donelson R. (2014) [1983]. Group dynamics (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. ISBN9781133956532. OCLC826872491.

*^Sparks, Erin (2007). 'Satisficing'. In Baumeister, Roy F.; Vohs, Kathleen D. (eds.). Encyclopedia of social psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. pp. 776–778. ISBN9781412916707. OCLC123119782.

*^Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. ISBN9780374275631. OCLC706020998.

*^ abKatsenelinboigen, Aron (1997). The concept of indeterminism and its applications: economics, social systems, ethics, artificial intelligence, and aesthetics(PDF). Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN978-0275957889. OCLC36438766. Archived from the original(PDF) on 2011-07-23. Retrieved 2015-07-27.

*^Ulea, Vera (2002). A concept of dramatic genre and the comedy of a new type: chess, literature, and film. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. pp. 17–18. ISBN978-0809324521. OCLC51301095.

*^Myers, Isabel Briggs; Kirby, Linda K.; Myers, Katharine D. (1998) [1976]. Introduction to type: a guide to understanding your results on the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator. Introduction to type series (6th ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. OCLC40336039.

*^Pittenger, David J. (2005). 'Cautionary comments regarding the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator'. Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research. 57 (3): 210–221. doi:10.1037/1065-9293.57.3.210.

*^Hogan, Robert (2007). Personality and the fate of organizations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 28. ISBN978-0805841428. OCLC65400436. Most personality psychologists regard the MBTI as little more than an elaborate Chinese fortune cookie..

*^Martinsons, Maris G. (December 2006). 'Comparing the decision styles of American, Chinese and Japanese business leaders'. Best Paper Proceedings of Academy of Management Meetings, Washington, DC, August 2001. SSRN952292.

*^Scott, Susanne G.; Bruce, Reginald A. (1995). 'Decision-making style: the development and assessment of a new measure'. Educational and Psychological Measurement. 55 (5): 818–831. doi:10.1177/0013164495055005017.

*^Thunholm, Peter (March 2004). 'Decision-making style: habit, style or both?'. Personality and Individual Differences. 36 (4): 931–944. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00162-4.Retrieved from 'https://en.wikipedia.org/w/index.php?title=Decision-making&oldid=910224955'

 

 

 

 

broken image